Skip to main content
menu sluit menu
Home Home
Login
Main navigation
  • Library
  • Calendar
  • e-Learning
  • News
    • Veterinary News In this section you find veterinary news
    • Recent Additions All content that was recently added to the IVIS library
  • Get involved
    • Donate Support IVIS, make a donation today
    • Media kit Promote your e-learning & events on IVIS
    • Add your e-learning & events to the IVIS calendar
    • Publish on IVIS Publish your work with us
  • About
    • Mission Our Mission Statement
    • What we do More info about IVIS and what we do
    • Who we are More info about the IVIS team
    • Authors See list of all IVIS authors and editors
  • Contact
User tools menu
User tools menu
Main navigation
  • Library
  • Calendar
  • e-Learning
  • News
    • Veterinary News In this section you find veterinary news
    • Recent Additions All content that was recently added to the IVIS library
  • Get involved
    • Donate Support IVIS, make a donation today
    • Media kit Promote your e-learning & events on IVIS
    • Add your e-learning & events to the IVIS calendar
    • Publish on IVIS Publish your work with us
  • About
    • Mission Our Mission Statement
    • What we do More info about IVIS and what we do
    • Who we are More info about the IVIS team
    • Authors See list of all IVIS authors and editors
  • Contact
Follow IVIS
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
Support IVIS

Breadcrumb

  1. Home
  2. Library
  3. Recent Advances in Companion Animal Behavior Problems
  4. Equine Stereotypies
Recent Advances in Companion Animal Behavior Problems - Houpt K.A.
Back to Table of Contents
Add to My Library
Close
Would you like to add this to your library?

Get access to all handy features included in the IVIS website

  • Get unlimited access to books, proceedings and journals.
  • Get access to a global catalogue of meetings, on-site and online courses, webinars and educational videos.
  • Bookmark your favorite articles in My Library for future reading.
  • Save future meetings and courses in My Calendar and My e-Learning.
  • Ask authors questions and read what others have to say.
Sign in Register
Comments
Print this article
Share:
  • Facebook
  • LinkedIn
  • Mail
  • Twitter

Equine Stereotypies

Author(s):
Nicol C.J.
In: Recent Advances in Companion Animal Behavior Problems by Houpt K.
Updated:
AUG 07, 2000
Languages:
  • EN
Back to Table of Contents
Add to My Library
Close
Would you like to add this to your library?

Get access to all handy features included in the IVIS website

  • Get unlimited access to books, proceedings and journals.
  • Get access to a global catalogue of meetings, on-site and online courses, webinars and educational videos.
  • Bookmark your favorite articles in My Library for future reading.
  • Save future meetings and courses in My Calendar and My e-Learning.
  • Ask authors questions and read what others have to say.
Sign in Register
Print this article
SHARE:
  • Facebook
  • LinkedIn
  • Mail
  • Twitter
    Read

    Equine stereotypies such as weaving, Crib-biting and box - walking are frequently described as "stable vices" and veterinary interest has generally focussed on the deleterious effects that may be associated with their performance. Many owners and veterinarians have pursued strategies to prevent the performance of stereotypies that include the use of physically restrictive devices, electric shock, and surgery [1]. These methods are, in many cases, ineffective [2], they may cause distress or harm to the horse, and they address symptoms rather than underlying causation. Pharmacological approaches to treatment have also been suggested. Dopamine pathways are intimately related to the appearance of stereotypic behaviour and opiate transmission regulates dopamine release, such that agonists and antagonists of enkephalin receptors can induce or suppress stereotypy [3]. This has led to studies looking at the efficacy of opioid antagonists as "treatments" for equine stereotypy. One study found transient elimination of crib-biting in all subjects treated with naloxone, nalmefene or diprenorphine [4]. Another study reported that weaving was reduced by administration of an opiate antagonist, although a serotonin transport inhibitor, and a neuroleptic were also effective [5]. The difficulty with such findings is that these pharmaceutical agents have many additional effects e.g. naloxone is a cataleptic and an appetite suppressant, and may reduce the performance of other functional behavioural systems. The effects of such drugs on the horse’s perception of its environment and on its overall welfare are not known. Of crucial significance to all attempts to treat the symptoms rather than the causes of stereotypy is recent work that suggests that some deleterious "effects" of stereotypy (e.g. digestive disorders, colic) may in reality reflect underlying problems that the horse is attempting to ameliorate by the performance of the stereotypy. In such cases, attempts to prevent the behaviour could do more harm than good. This dilemma for the veterinarian emphasizes the need to understand more about the causes and functions of equine stereotypies.

    Description

    Crib-biting

    The horse seizes a fixed object with its incisors and pulls back, drawing air into its cranial oesophagus, and usually emitting a characteristic grunt. As the behaviour develops it may become more complex in form, sometimes involving licking before or after each bite. The behaviour is strikingly invariant for any given horse, and may occur in just one or two locations. Although probably observed most frequently within a stable, it is also quite common for horses to crib-bite on the fence while out at pasture. Crib-biting frequently occurs during or just after the provision of a concentrate meal and may, in some horses, be intimately patterned within bouts of forage ingestion. Contrary to a widely held belief, crib-biting does not involve aerophagia, as inhaled air is expelled again via the mouth [6].

    Wind-sucking

    The physical form of the behaviour is almost identical to Crib-biting but the horse achieves the same posture and tension of the neck muscles without grasping a fixed object.

    Weaving

    The horse sways its head laterally, sometimes involving the neck, forequarters and sometimes hindquarters. Weaving usually occurs while the horse is standing with the head over the stable door, although it can be performed in any part of the stable, or by a field gate.

    Distribution of Equine Stereotypies

    Six surveys have been conducted in recent years to discover the prevalence of stereotypic behaviour in a variety of equine populations around the world [7, [12]. These studies were cross-sectional in nature i.e. they asked about stereotypy prevalence and management practices at the time of the survey only. Overall prevalence rates between populations were surprisingly similar. Approximately 12% of adult horses wood-chew, 4% crib-bite, 3% weave and 2% box-walk. Despite the overall similarities, some studies reported significantly higher stereotypy prevalences in Thoroughbred populations compared to other breeds, in dressage and event horses compared to endurance horses, and in stallions compared to mares. Genetic factors may play a part. Selection experiments in rodents have shown that the propensity of the F2 generation to develop stereotypies is markedly related to the occurrence of stereotypies in their parents [13]. In horses, certain Thoroughbred bloodlines appear more likely to produce stereotypic individuals than others [14] although it is difficult to exclude environmental or management differences. Management factors certainly vary between populations. Thus, Thoroughbreds received more concentrate feed and had fewer opportunities for social contact than Swedish Trotters [11] whilst dressage and event horses spent more time in their stables than endurance horses [8]. It is therefore possible to assess the effect of the management factors only by looking within a relatively uniform genetic population. This has been done for Thoroughbred horses.

    Management Factors

    In the UK Thoroughbred horse population, positive associations have been reported between overall stereotypy prevalence and stable designs that limited the degree of social contact between horses, the use of non-straw bedding, and a forage ration of less than 6.8kg per day [9]. In Swedish Thoroughbreds, a similar correlation was noted between stereotypy prevalence and low forage ration. In addition, high concentrate feed rations were positively associated with stereotypy prevalence [11]. Stereotypy prevalences were lower in Australian Thoroughbred horses kept at pasture compared with stabled horses [12]. This statistical information is important as it suggests that some management practices may cause stereotypic behaviour to occur. However, cross-sectional survey data cannot reveal whether management practices precede or follow the onset of stereotypic behaviour for any particular horse, or even whether stereotypic horses are bought in from other yards with different management practices. In addition, very large datasets are required to draw valid statistical conclusions, and few of the surveys conducted have been able to examine the separate risk factors associated with the different forms of equine stereotypy. For these reasons prospective studies, where horses are monitored over time are a more powerful source of information. One study in the UK followed 218 foals over a 4 year period [15]. In this study the prevalences of box-walking and weaving were similar to other surveys but Crib-biting was observed in 10.5% of the horses and occurred at a strikingly early age. The median age of onset of crib-biting was only 20 weeks, in contrast to a median age of onset of weaving at 60 weeks. Thus, in many cases foals started Crib-biting before they were weaned whilst still out on pasture with their dams. The only management intervention that these young animals had received was the introduction of concentrate feeds. Some foals had been allowed to nibble their mothers’ rations from a few days of age. Others were given concentrate feeds shortly before weaning to familiarise them with a new diet and decrease the risk of a slowing of growth at weaning. Even after weaning, foals that received concentrate feeds were at four times the risk of developing Crib-biting than foals that did not receive concentrate feeds [15].

    Why do Horses Start to Crib-Bite?

    The epidemiological data suggest that low forage and high concentrate rations are risk factors for Crib-biting. It has been suggested that a high forage ration may reduce the time available to perform stereotypy or that the motivation to forage is reduced through increased gut - fill or other feedback mechanisms [15,16]. However, recent data suggest another mechanism. It has been known for some time that the feeding of concentrate diets [17] and periods of food deprivation increase gastric acidity to harmful levels that can result in rapid ulceration [18]. High concentrate diets also alter caecal fermentation and increase caecal acidity [19]. In one study, early signs of abnormal wood - chewing and stable biting were higher in horses fed a predominantly concentrate diet than horses fed 8 Kg of hay per day but not if the horses fed concentrates were given an additional supplement, virginiamycin, that suppressed lactic acid production in the hindgut and increased hindgut pH [20]. This suggests that Crib-biting may function to reduce acidity of the digestive tract. The exact mechanism by which Crib-biting could ameliorate excessive acidity of digestive tract is not known but it has been suggested that Crib-biting may result in increased salivary flow [21]. Equine saliva is a natural buffer but it is produced only during mastication and not in response to the sight or smell of food [22]. Horses fed low fibre rations, or placed in situations where calm, continuous forage intake is unlikely (e.g. individually weaned foals are at a greater risk of developing stereotypies than group - weaned foals [15]) may simply not produce sufficient saliva to buffer their stomach contents. The tension exerted during the Crib-biting action may be an alternative method of stimulating saliva production, although this hypothesis remains to be rigorously tested.

    Why do Horses Start to Weave?

    In the UK many horses start to weave when they are about one year old, after they have been sold from their birthplace stud and re-stabled in new surroundings [15]. Casual observations that horses perform weaving over the stable door, particularly when companions from neighbouring stables are removed suggest that horses may start to weave when their inclination to leave the stable (particularly to regain contact with other horses) is thwarted. Weaving develops as a form of frustrated escape response. The epidemiological data partly supports this interpretation as stable designs that reduce social isolation also reduce the risk of stereotypic behaviour (including weaving) [8,10]. However, a recent experiment provides further support [23]. Weaving horses were housed in stables where they could see out only at the front, or additionally at the back or the sides, allowing a greater degree of contact with neighbouring horses. The incidence of weaving was significantly reduced when the horses had more opportunities for social interaction with their neighbours, and dropped to zero when all four windows were open [23]. A pilot study showed that placing mirrors in the stable may also reduce the frequency of weaving behaviour [21] although this requires verification.

    Stereotypies and Stress

    An influential general hypothesis proposes that stereotypy functions to reduce stress experienced in captive environments. There have been a number of attempts to determine whether stereotypy assists horses to "cope" in this way, but it is difficult to obtain proof. A key prediction is that stereotypic animals should have lower levels of stress either all the time, or at least during bouts of stereotypy, than non - stereotypic horses exposed to the same environment. Researchers have investigated heart rate, plasma cortisol and plasma beta - endorphin but have obtained sharply conflicting results. Some studies have found no difference in baseline levels between stereotypic and normal horses [24-26], while others have found either higher [27] or lower [28] plasma cortisol or endorphin concentrations in stereotypic horses. One difficulty is that stereotypic horses may have been more reactive than other horses before they developed a stereotypy. The onset of stereotypy may serve only to reduce their stress levels to a "normal" level, but this could be determined only if long - term prospective studies were initiated prior to stereotypy development.

    There is some evidence that heart rate is lowered in horses during bouts of cribbing behaviour compared with other behaviours [25, [29] but, in general, evidence for the coping hypothesis in horses is weak.

    Mature Horses and Established Stereotypies

    Instead of reflecting current problems, adult stereotypy may be like a "scar" representing a severe problem experienced at the time of onset [30]. Although this has not been investigated in horses, it is known that wild - caught rodents (that presumably did not experience frustrating or stressful conditions when young) rarely show stereotypic behaviour in barren cages, whereas their laboratory reared offspring do [31]. Improving the environment can reduce stereotypic behaviour in young rodents but not to any great extent in older animals [28]. This suggests that young animals are particularly sensitive to conditions that elicit stereotypic behaviour. It is generally thought that, with age, stereotypies are elicited by a wider set of stimuli than in early development (emancipation) and become relatively immune to normal control mechanisms. It should therefore come as no surprise to see older horses performing stereotypies in environments that appear to support high welfare. In older horses stereotypies may also become conditioned responses, activated in anticipation of a particular eliciting event (a concentrate meal, removal of a companion) as a protective response. It may not be possible to reverse the process of stereotypy development in mature animals but the proportion of time they spend performing the stereotypy can clearly be manipulated by increasing opportunities for social contact [23] and foraging behaviour [32].

    Conclusion

    Prevention of stereotypic behaviour is a better strategy than control. Young foals and horses are at particular risk but this risk can be reduced by maintaining opportunities for social contact particularly during and immediately after weaning, feeding high forage rations and providing the minimum amount of concentrate feed to sustain health.

    Environmental improvement should be the first step in managing horses with established stereotypies and caution should be exercised if other preventive methods are considered so as not to compromise the horses’ welfare [1]

    Back to Table of Contents
    Add to My Library
    Close
    Would you like to add this to your library?

    Get access to all handy features included in the IVIS website

    • Get unlimited access to books, proceedings and journals.
    • Get access to a global catalogue of meetings, on-site and online courses, webinars and educational videos.
    • Bookmark your favorite articles in My Library for future reading.
    • Save future meetings and courses in My Calendar and My e-Learning.
    • Ask authors questions and read what others have to say.
    Sign in Register
    Print this article
    References

    1.  McGreevy PD, Nicol CJ. Prevention of crib-biting: a review. Eq Vet J Suppl 1998; 27:35-38. - PubMed -

    ...
    Show all
    Comments (0)

    Ask the author

    0 comments
    Submit
    Close
    Would to like to further discuss this item?

    Get access to all handy features included in the IVIS website

    • Get unlimited access to books, proceedings and journals.
    • Get access to a global catalogue of meetings, on-site and online courses, webinars and educational videos.
    • Bookmark your favorite articles in My Library for future reading.
    • Save future meetings and courses in My Calendar and My e-Learning.
    • Ask authors questions and read what others have to say.
    Sign in Register
    About

    How to reference this publication (Harvard system)?

    Nicol, C. (2000) “Equine Stereotypies”, Recent Advances in Companion Animal Behavior Problems. Available at: https://www.ivis.org/library/recent-advances-companion-animal-behavior-problems/equine-stereotypies (Accessed: 30 March 2023).

    Affiliation of the authors at the time of publication

    Division of Animal Health and Husbandry, Department of Clinical Veterinary Science, Langford House, Langford, North Somerset, United Kingdom.

    Author(s)

    • Professor Christine Nicol

      Nicol C.J.

      Senior Research Associate
      DVM PhD
      Department of Clinical Veterinary Science, Division of Animal Health and Husbandry, University of Bristol
      Read more about this author

    Copyright Statement

    © All text and images in this publication are copyright protected and cannot be reproduced or copied in any way.
    Related Content

    Readers also viewed these publications

    • Proceeding

      BEVA - Annual Congress - Liverpool, 2022

      By: British Equine Veterinary Association
      MAR 20, 2023
    • Journal Issue

      Veterinary Evidence - Vol 8 N°1, Jan-Mar 2023

      In: Veterinary Evidence
      MAR 19, 2023
    • Proceeding

      AVEF - Conférence Annuelle - Reims, 2022

      By: Association des Vétérinaires Équins Français
      MAR 03, 2023
    • Proceeding

      EEHNC - Virtual Congress - 2021

      By: European Equine Health and Nutrition Congress
      FEB 09, 2023
    • Proceeding

      SFT - Theriogenology Annual Conference - Bellevue, 2022

      By: Society for Theriogenology
      JAN 10, 2023
    • Proceeding

      ACVIM & ECEIM - Consensus Statements

      By: American College of Veterinary Internal Medicine
      NOV 11, 2022
    • Journal Issue

      Veterinary Evidence - Vol 7 N°2, Apr-Jun 2022

      In: Veterinary Evidence
      OCT 07, 2022
    • Journal Issue

      Veterinary Evidence - Vol 7 N°3, Jul-Sep 2022

      In: Veterinary Evidence
      OCT 04, 2022
    • Journal Issue

      Veterinary Practice Management Articles - Veterinary Focus

      In: Veterinary Focus
      AUG 05, 2022
    • Chapter

      Nutrition

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      JUL 09, 2022
    • Chapter

      Pharmacology and Therapeutics

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      JUL 03, 2022
    • Chapter

      Sedation, Anaesthesia and Analgesia

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      JUN 05, 2022
    • Chapter

      The Geriatric Donkey

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      MAY 20, 2022
    • Chapter

      Euthanasia and the Post-Mortem Examination

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      MAY 20, 2022
    • Chapter

      Appendix 7: Example Diets: for the mature, pregnant and lactating donkey

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      MAY 12, 2022
    • Chapter

      Appendix 2: Donkey Weight Estimator

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      MAY 12, 2022
    • Chapter

      Appendix 1: The Clinical Examination

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      MAY 12, 2022
    • Chapter

      Appendix 5: Monitoring your Donkey’s Quality of Life

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      MAY 12, 2022
    • Chapter

      Appendix 6: Professional record of Assessment for Quality of Life

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      MAY 12, 2022
    • Chapter

      Appendix 3: Body Condition Scoring

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      MAY 12, 2022
    • Chapter

      Appendix 4: Parameters: Biochemistry and Haematology

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      MAY 12, 2022
    • Chapter

      The Care of the Foal

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      MAY 12, 2022
    • Chapter

      Approach to the Dull Donkey

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      MAY 07, 2022
    • Chapter

      Parasitology

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      APR 26, 2022
    • Chapter

      The Foot

      In: The Clinical Companion of the Donkey (2nd Edition)
      APR 06, 2022
    • Load more
    Back To Top
    Become a member of IVIS and get access to all our resources
    Create an account
    Sign in
    Leading the way in providing veterinary information
    About IVIS
    • Mission
    • What we do
    • Who we are
    Need help?
    • Contact
    Follow IVIS
    • Twitter
    • Facebook
    International Veterinary Information Service (IVIS) is a not-for-profit organization established to provide information to veterinarians, veterinary students, technicians and animal health professionals worldwide using Internet technology.
    Support IVIS
    © 2023 International Veterinary Information Service
    • Disclaimer
    • Privacy Policy